Size-selective harvesting fosters adaptations in mating behaviour and reproductive allocation, affecting sexual selection in fish
Funding information
VS was supported by a Leibniz-DAAD Postdoctoral Research Fellowship (no. 91632699). DB received financial support by the DFG (BI 1828/2-1). SUH was funded by AXA Research Fund and The Finnish Cultural Foundation. AEH was supported by a University of Minnesota Doctoral Dissertation Fellowship.
Abstract
- The role of sexual selection in the context of harvest-induced evolution is poorly understood. However, elevated and trait-selective harvesting of wild populations may change sexually selected traits, which in turn can affect mate choice and reproduction.
- We experimentally evaluated the potential for fisheries-induced evolution of mating behaviour and reproductive allocation in fish.
- We used an experimental system of zebrafish (Danio rerio) lines exposed to large, small or random (i.e. control) size-selective mortality. The large-harvested line represented a treatment simulating the typical case in fisheries where the largest individuals are preferentially harvested. We used a full factorial design of spawning trials with size-matched individuals to control for the systematic impact of body size during reproduction, thereby singling out possible changes in mating behaviour and reproductive allocation.
- Both small size-selective mortality and large size-selective mortality left a legacy on male mating behaviour by elevating intersexual aggression. However, there was no evidence for line-assortative reproductive allocation. Females of all lines preferentially allocated eggs to the generally less aggressive males of the random-harvested control line. Females of the large-harvested line showed enhanced reproductive performance, and males of the large-harvested line had the highest egg fertilization rate among all males. These findings can be explained as an evolutionary adaptation by which individuals of the large-harvested line display an enhanced reproductive performance early in life to offset the increased probability of adult mortality due to harvest.
- Our results suggest that the large-harvested line evolved behaviourally mediated reproductive adaptations that could increase the rate of recovery when populations adapted to high fishing pressure come into secondary contact with other populations.
1 INTRODUCTION
Many wildlife and fish populations are intensively harvested by humans. Harvesting represents an unnatural environmental pressure that has the potential to strongly shape the fitness landscape in exploited populations (Jørgensen et al., 2007). Indeed, exploitation of both fish and wildlife populations has been found to induce evolutionary adaptations in life-history and other traits over relatively short timescales (Alberti et al., 2017; Heino, Díaz Pauli, & Dieckmann, 2015; Kuparinen & Festa-Bianchet, 2017).
Fishing has been described as a large-scale experiment in life-history evolution (Rijnsdorp, 1993) because it can be intensive and often size-selective (Jørgensen et al., 2007; Kuparinen & Festa-Bianchet, 2017; Law, 2007). Research on fisheries-induced evolution (FIE) has mainly focused on the evolution of life-history traits (Devine, Wright, Pardoe, & Heino, 2012; Heino et al., 2015; Sharpe & Hendry, 2009). However, size-selective harvesting can also alter the physiology and behaviour of fish populations (Arlinghaus et al., 2017; Hollins et al., 2018; Walsh, Munch, Chiba, & Conover, 2006), including mate choice patters (Sordalen et al., 2018). Because nearly all phenotypic traits are at least modestly heritable (Dochtermann, Schwab, & Sih, 2015; Mousseau & Roff, 1987; Roff, 2002), harvest-induced trait change has the potential to foster the evolution of divergent mate choice patterns in exploited populations, which in turn can affect reproductive output (Dunlop, Shuter, & Dieckmann, 2007; Rowe & Hutchings, 2003; Sordalen et al., 2018). Indeed, selective harvesting can directionally change sexually selected traits (e.g. Chiyo, Obanda, & Korir, 2015, Pigeon, Festa-Bianchet, Coltman, & Pelletier, 2016, Sordalen et al., 2018), but the role of sexual selection in FIE remains largely unexplored (but see Sordalen et al., 2018).
The fitness landscapes created by intensive and trait-selective harvesting could foster local adaptations in a manner similar to that of ecological gradients in eco-evolutionary contexts. Such scenarios are conceivable if meta-populations are exposed to differential fishing pressure and there is limited or no gene flow among subpopulations (Alós et al., 2014). As such, the evolution of subpopulation-specific trait in response to selection gradients (e.g. those caused by different exploitation pressures) could even foster reproductive isolation barriers through sexual selection (Hendry, 2004; Hendry, Nosil, & Rieseberg, 2007; Nosil, Vines, & Funk, 2005). Local adaptation can be facilitated by “magic” traits that are sexually selected and contribute to non-random mating, reproductive isolation and eventually speciation (Gavrilets, 2004; Servedio, Doorn, Kopp, Frame, & Nosil, 2011). The contribution of sexual selection to early stages of ecological speciation has been demonstrated in several studies in which mate choice in fishes was affected by multiple ecological factors (Endler & Houde, 1995; Langerhans, Gifford, & Joseph, 2007; Seehausen et al., 2008). One of these factors is mortality associated with different predation regimes, which has for example been found to be responsible for reproductive isolation driven by assortative mate choice in Gambusia (Langerhans et al., 2007). It is plausible that mortality induced by fishing can produce similar evolutionary outcomes by favouring reproductive isolation driven by traits that are adapted to selective harvesting in different spatial units.
Sexual selection could also exert strong effects on FIE of phenotypic traits (Hutchings & Rowe, 2008; Lane, Forrest, & Willis, 2011; Rowe & Hutchings, 2003). In particular, phenotype-driven changes in the mating system (e.g. mate choice, dominance hierarchies or intrasexual competition; Lane et al., 2011) induced by intense fishing pressure and associated phenotypic changes in sexually selected traits (e.g. body size or behaviour) could affect (a) the outcome of FIE; (b) the speed, magnitude and direction of evolving traits; and (c) population recovery (Dunlop et al., 2007; Enberg, Jørgensen, Dunlop, Heino, & Dieckmann, 2009; Hutchings & Rowe, 2008). For example, Sordalen et al. (2018) demonstrated that size-selective fishing of large male European lobsters (Homarus gammarus)—the phenotype that naturally offers large fitness advantages—has the potential to attenuate sexual selection in fished areas, with a subsequent acceleration of FIE towards small male sizes. The authors suggest the creation of marine reserves to strengthen sexual selection and allow spillover of large males to exploited populations, so as to buffer FIE in exploited areas (Sordalen et al., 2018). We are not aware of any empirical studies that address the effects of size-selective harvesting on sexual selection which in turn can affect reproductive performance in exploited fish populations.
We explored whether harvest-induced selection can foster the evolution of reproductive behaviour using an experimentally controlled system of zebrafish (Danio rerio) lines exposed to positive and negative size-selective harvesting (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). In earlier studies, the zebrafish selection lines have been found to differ phenotypically in reproductive allocation (defined as the amount of energy allocated to reproduction; Lester, Shuter, & Abrams, 2004), post-maturation growth and adult body size (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). Moreover, the selection lines differ genetically, in gene expression profiles and in personality traits (Sbragaglia, Alós, et al., 2019; Uusi-Heikkilä, Savilammi, Leder, Arlinghaus, & Primmer, 2017; Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). In particular, the line in which large individuals were harvested (simulated trawl-like selectivity patterns or harvest regulations mimicking minimum-landing sizes) evolved a smaller body length, earlier age at maturity, higher relative fecundity and a smaller maximum length compared to the control, suggesting evolution towards a fast life history (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). In contrast, the line in which small individuals were harvested (simulating a harvest regulation mimicking a maximum-size limit or a gear with sharply reduced selectivity for very large individuals) evolved not only a different maturation schedule, but also reduced reproductive allocation compared to controls while maintaining adult sizes similar to the controls, suggestive of a slow life history (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015).
The fact that size-selective harvesting changed both adult size and behaviour in the zebrafish model system is relevant from an assortative mating perspective. To isolate the behaviour-driven mechanisms of mate choice, it is important to control for size because it is a key trait of sexual selection in zebrafish and other fishes (Nasiadka & Clark, 2012; Pyron, 2003; Uusi-Heikkilä, Böckenhoff, Wolter, & Arlinghaus, 2012). Female zebrafish allocate reproductive resources depending on male size, usually favouring large and dominant males (Pyron, 2003; Skinner & Watt, 2007; Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, Wolter, Meinelt, & Arlinghaus, 2012). However, the reproductive allocation of female zebrafish may be reduced in the presence of extremely large males, probably due to an overt dominance and continued sexual harassment of females that is reproductively costly (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2018; Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, et al., 2012). In fact, an important component for successful courtship behaviour in zebrafish is the chemically mediated inhibition of male aggression by the release of ovarian pheromones by females (Hurk & Lambert, 1983; Spence, Gerlach, Lawrence, & Smith, 2008). In addition to size, male personality traits, including boldness and aggression, have been shown to affect offspring production and mate choice patterns in both zebrafish (Ariyomo & Watt, 2012; Vargas, Mackenzie, & Rey, 2018) and other teleosts (Ariyomo & Watt, 2013; Bierbach, Wolf, Sommer-Trembo, Hanisch, & Plath, 2015; Ibarra-Zatarain, Parati, Cenadelli, & Duncan, 2019). In zebrafish, bold and aggressive (i.e. proactive) individuals have been found to reproductively outperform shy and less aggressive (i.e. reactive) fishes, independent of coloration patterns (Vargas et al., 2018). In guppies (Poecilia reticulata), females with similar personality to males have shown an elevated probability of producing offspring (Ariyomo & Watt, 2013). Accordingly, fisheries-induced changes in reproductive behaviour could affect reproductive output and lead to subpopulation-assortative mating, thereby contributing to fisheries-induced evolution of mate choice and possibly reproductive isolation.
To investigate whether size-selective harvesting can promote the evolution of mating behaviour, we conducted size-matched spawning trials among the aforementioned lines of zebrafish. We examined the hypothesis that size selection has altered the mating behaviour of both size-selected lines and that these changes create a preferential mate choice for members of their own selection line. Accordingly, we predicted that during dyadic intersexual encounters (one male and one female), male aggression would be lower when both individuals were from the same selection line. Moreover, we expected that males of the line where small individuals were harvested would display lower levels of aggression and that males of the line where large individuals were harvested would display higher levels of aggression due to the correlation between life-history traits and aggression (i.e. the pace-of-life syndrome; Réale et al., 2010, Dammhahn, Dingemanse, Niemelä, & Réale, 2018). We focused our predictions on males because previous research has shown that male behaviour, particularly male aggression during spawning, is of key importance in zebrafish mating behaviour (Ibarra-Zatarain et al., 2019; Spence et al., 2008; Vargas et al., 2018). We further predicted that patterns of line-assortative mate choice would manifest in greater numbers of eggs released and fertilized when zebrafish were crossed within the same selection line relative to being paired with any of the other two selection lines.
2 MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1 Zebrafish selection lines
We used individuals from the F11 generation of the selection lines from Uusi-Heikkilä et al. (2015). We applied a strong directional selection pressures (a 75% per-generation harvest rate) acting on either large body size (large fishes harvested, as is typical for many fisheries; hereafter referred to as large-harvested line) or small body size (small fishes harvested as is possible in recreational fisheries, Pierce, 2010, or in fisheries managed with a maximum-length limit; hereafter referred to as small-harvested line). We also applied a selection pressure randomly with respect to size (hereafter referred to as random-harvested line), representing a harvested control (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). Size selection occurred during the first five generations, after which harvesting halted for six generations to remove any maternal effects and to facilitate the study of the evolutionary outcomes of selection using a common-garden approach (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). We firstly conducted analyses of life-history and lifetime growth at F9 revealing that males and females in the large-harvested line evolved a smaller adult length and weight (Figure 1) and higher relative fecundity compared to controls (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). By contrast, the small-harvested line showed reduced reproductive allocation and no change in adult length compared to the control line (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). Both size-selected lines evolved an alteration of the maturation schedule and matured at smaller sizes and younger ages than fish of the control line (for more details see Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). These findings suggest a harvest-induced change in energy allocation. Any changes in basal life-history traits related to growth and maturation affect the post-maturation growth trajectory (Enberg et al., 2012). Although evolutionary rebound of key life-history traits from F5 to F11 could happen and is reported in other selection experiments with different species (Conover, Munch, & Arnott, 2009; Salinas et al., 2012), among-generation assays of the growth trajectory using a Lester biphasic growth model (Honsey, Staples, & Venturelli, 2017; Lester et al., 2004; Wilson, Honsey, Moe, Venturelli, & Reynolds, 2018) conducted at F9 and F13 (see the Appendix S1 for details) showed that the selection lines maintained the evolved differences in life history and terminal length until F13 (Figure 1). Thus, the evolved differences were almost certainly maintained at F11 (the generation used in the present work). Accordingly, we considered the large-harvested line to be characterized by a fast life history and small terminal size compared to the control, and the small-harvested line by a slow life history and a similar terminal size compared to the control.
We raised individuals from the F11 generation in separate tanks in a common recirculation system until adulthood (~120 days post-fertilization), after which we separated them according to sex-related morphological traits (Nasiadka & Clark, 2012). Then, we anesthetized the fish with ethylene glycol monophenyl ether (Merck KGaA) at a dilution of 50–75 mg/L, placed them in a Petri dish for measuring with a millimetre paper. After that we transferred the fish into acclimation aquaria (50 × 50 × 50 cm; water level 40 cm, no substrate) for at least three days before experimental trials. We used size-matched females ranging from 32 to 34 mm (standard length: mean ± SD; 33.5 ± 0.5 mm) and males ranging from 30 to 32 mm (standard length: 31 ± 0.5 mm), to single out a possible impact of size selection on behaviour and subsequent mating within and among lines. We used males that were smaller than females because previous findings indicate that there are substantial reproductive costs related to males being too large relative to females (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2018; Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, et al., 2012).
2.2 Male aggression during intersexual dyadic contests
To understand whether size selection affected male behaviour during spawning contexts, we created a full factorial design and examined all mating combinations among the three selection lines. We used eight replicates for each of the nine combinations. We randomly selected previously unfamiliar individuals (one male and one female) from the acclimation aquaria and housed them in five-litre spawning boxes. We placed the boxes in the same recirculating water system under the following conditions: water temperature at 26 ± 0.5°C; 10–14-hr light–darkness cycle (light on at 07:00 am) and feeding with dry food (TetraMin, Tetra) and Artemia nauplii (Inve Aquaculture). We separated each box from the others by a white plastic divider to prevent visual contact. Additionally, we installed a cover shield at the front of each box that allowed scoring of behaviour of one box without disturbing the others. To that end, we raised the divider and counted the number of agonistic interactions, that is bites (i.e. biting is the most common aggressive behaviour in zebrafish; Paull et al., 2010). We counted the number of bites during five minutes after the first bite occurred. Within a given box, the two individuals were kept separated by a sponge for the first 24 hr. Agonistic interactions were then observed between 09:00 and 13:00.
2.3 Group spawning trials and reproductive allocation
To study outcomes of mate choice on reproductive allocation, we carried out a 4-day spawning trial between two females and four males, an assay that was used previously on these lines (e.g. Uusi-Heikkilä, Böckenhoff, et al., 2012; Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, et al., 2012; Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). We used seven replicates for each of the nine combinations resulting from the full factorial design among the three selection lines. We used each group of fish only once, and we did not include the individuals already used for the aggression trials. The spawning trials took place in five-litre spawning boxes placed in the same recirculating water system following the conditions reported above. Spawning in zebrafish occurs during the first daylight hours (Darrow & Harris, 2004). Therefore, we removed eggs from the spawning boxes every day at 10:00 a.m. for four consecutive days, as was done in previous studies with these lines (e.g. Uusi-Heikkilä, Böckenhoff, et al., 2012; Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, et al., 2012). We counted any collected eggs under a stereomicroscope and sorted them into fertilized and non-fertilized groups. We assumed that the total number of eggs released represented differential allocation by female zebrafish to preferred/non-preferred males (Skinner & Watt, 2007; Spence & Smith, 2006; Uusi-Heikkilä, Böckenhoff, et al., 2012), and we used the total number of fertilized eggs as a metric of reproductive fitness. We also recorded the fertilization rate, which can be affected by differential allocation of high- or low-quality eggs to males by females (Uusi-Heikkilä, Böckenhoff, et al., 2012), by sperm quality or by the ability of males to fertilize eggs (e.g. Watt, Skinner, Hale, Nakagawa, & Burke, 2011).
2.4 Statistical analysis
We analysed the response variables with separate generalized linear models using a quasipoisson distribution for male aggression, total number of eggs and number of fertilized eggs, and a quasibinomial distribution for the proportion of fertilized eggs in relation to all eggs. We used quasi-distributions to account for overdispersion of the data. We tested for the effects of two categorical independent variables—female line and male line—as main effects and interactions. Given the hypothesis-testing nature of our study, we used an information theoretic approach (Johnson & Omland, 2004). We selected the best fitting model using second-order quasi-Akaike information criteria for small sample sizes (qAICc) and corresponding model weights (Richards, Whittingham, & Stephens, 2011). Additionally, we used adjusted R2 to quantify the proportion of explained variance by the independent variables. We ran all analyses using r version 3.4.3 (www.R-project.org/) with the additional package “MuMIn” (Bartoń, 2014) for model selection and “rsq” (Zhang, 2018) for calculating R2 (Data are available here: Sbragaglia, Gliese, et al., 2019).
3 RESULTS
Males displayed strong variation in number of bites, ranging from 0 to 181 during the 5-min trials. Reproductive performance was also highly variable in terms of total number of eggs (from 1 to 1,246), number of eggs fertilized (from 0 to 380) and proportion of fertilized eggs (from 0 to 0.69).
Male aggression during intersexual dyadic contests was best described by the full model including the interaction between female line and male line (Mod1 in Table 1). Male aggression and variability in male aggression were lowest when females from the small- and random-harvested lines were crossed with males of the random-harvested line (Figure 2a). By contrast, male aggression was much higher when females from the random-harvested line were crossed with males from the large- and small-harvested lines, although variation was large (Figure 2a). Despite substantial variation within each line, females from the large-harvested line experienced a lower level of male aggression than females from the other two lines (Figure 3a). In addition, males from both the large- and small-harvested lines were generally more aggressive than males from the random-harvested line (Figure 3b). These results suggest that, in contrast to our hypothesis, size selection altered male aggression in the same direction in both selection lines.
Response variable | Models | qAICc | D | W | Adj. R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male aggression | Mod1: F line × M line | 99.2 | 0.00 | 0.73 | 0.16 |
Mod2: F line + M line | 104.5 | 5.29 | 0.05 | 0.08 | |
Mod3: F line | 109.9 | 7.90 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Mod4: M line | 101.7 | 2.54 | 0.20 | 0.07 | |
Mod5: null model | 107.1 | 10.65 | 0.01 | – | |
Total number of eggs | Mod6: F line × M line | 83.6 | 5.37 | 0.04 | 0.15 |
Mod7: F line + M line | 78.2 | 0.00 | 0.60 | 0.15 | |
Mod8: F line | 81.7 | 3.47 | 0.11 | 0.10 | |
Mod9: M line | 80.3 | 2.06 | 0.21 | 0.07 | |
Mod10: null model | 83.7 | 5.52 | 0.04 | – | |
Number of fertilized eggs | Mod11: F line × M line | 86.7 | 10.58 | 0.00 | 0.05 |
Mod12: F line + M line | 77.2 | 1.05 | 0.22 | 0.09 | |
Mod13: F line | 77.6 | 1.48 | 0.18 | 0.06 | |
Mod14: M line | 76.1 | 0.00 | 0.37 | 0.03 | |
Mod15: null model | 77.1 | 0.99 | 0.23 | – | |
Proportion of fertilized eggs | Mod16: F line × M line | 94.4 | 13.29 | 0.0 | 0.18 |
Mod17: F line + M line | 87.8 | 4.73 | 0.09 | 0.20 | |
Mod18: F line | 103.8 | 20.67 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Mod19: M line | 83.1 | 0.00 | 0.91 | 0.23 | |
Mod20: null model | 99.6 | 16.50 | 0.00 | – |
Despite evolutionary adaptation of male spawning behaviour due to size selection, females did not release more eggs when paired with males of their own selection line, as indicated by the poor fit of the model that included an interaction term between female line and male line (Mod6 in Table 1; Figure 2b). Instead, the model with male and female lines as additive effects was the best model (Mod7 in Table 1). This finding did not support our hypothesis of assortative mate choice within zebrafish lines leading to line-assortative reproductive allocation. Moreover, there were substantial differences in the number of eggs released across lines, as revealed by the significant main effects of female and male lines (Table 1). Independently of which male was present, females from the large-harvested line released more eggs on average than females from the random-harvested line, while females from the small-harvested line released fewer eggs than females from the random-harvested line (Figure 3c). Furthermore, females from all lines released fewer eggs when crossed with the more aggressive males of the large- and small-harvested lines than with males of the random-harvested line (Figure 3d).
The total number of fertilized eggs and the percentage of fertilized eggs were also not systematically larger when males and females of the same line were paired together, as indicated by the poor fit of both models with interaction terms (Mod11 and Mod16 in Table 1; see also Figure 2c,d). This finding further reinforced the idea that there was no line-assortative reproductive allocation. Instead, the model with male line as the only explanatory variable performed best in explaining the total number of fertilized eggs, with the model with female line as an additional additive effect performing similarly well (Table 1). Specifically, males from the large-harvested line produced the largest number of fertilized eggs (Figure 3f). We also found a similar but weaker pattern in the females from the large-harvested line (Figure 3e). Finally, males of the large-harvested line fertilized a greater percentage of eggs than males of the small- and random-harvested lines (Mod19 in Table 1 and Figure 3h), independent of the female line with which they were crossed (Figure 3g).
4 DISCUSSION
We found that five generations of size-selective harvesting altered male aggression during intersexual dyadic encounters. However, we did not find evidence for assortative mating within zebrafish lines evolutionarily adapted to either large or small size-selective harvesting when engaging in size-matched spawning trials. Our results partially supported the first hypothesis that size-selective harvesting left a legacy in sexually selected behavioural traits. In particular, males of the random-harvested line were less aggressive when crossed with females of the small- and random-harvested lines, while females of the random-harvested line experienced considerably more male aggression when crossed with males of the large- and small-harvested lines. Our second hypothesis was not supported because we did not find evidence of line-assortative reproductive allocation. Instead, we found strong evidence for large- and small-harvested females releasing more eggs when paired with control (i.e. random-harvested) males. Moreover, our results showed that females from the large-harvested line produced more fertilized eggs, and males from the large-harvested lines fertilized a greater proportion of eggs, compared to the other two lines.
Previous studies showed that the zebrafish selection lines differed in personality traits such as exploration, boldness and sociability (Sbragaglia, Alós, et al., 2019; Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). Our work adds to this research by showcasing that size selection has altered male aggressive behaviour during intersexual dyadic contests. In contrast to expectations, male aggression increased in both size-selected lines and did not diverge as a result of the opposing directional size selection. The increased aggression by males of the large-harvested line agrees with the pace-of-life hypothesis (Dammhahn et al., 2018; Réale et al., 2010). Accordingly, the fast life history is selected to reap fitness benefits early in life, such that males of the large-harvested line become more aggressive than controls, which has previously been shown to enhance reproductive success in zebrafish (Ariyomo & Watt, 2012; Vargas et al., 2018). This interpretation cannot explain the high aggression level of males of the small-harvested line. We speculate instead that behavioural syndromes across life stages (i.e. correlations of traits such as boldness, exploration and aggression; Sih, Bell, & Johnson, 2004, Conrad, Weinersmith, Brodin, Saltz, & Sih, 2011) could explain why males in the small-harvested line, which were found to be bolder as juveniles (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015), were also found to be more aggressive than controls as adults.
Differential allocation of reproductive resources by females to preferred males has been repeatedly documented in zebrafish (e.g. Skinner & Watt, 2007, Ariyomo & Watt, 2012, Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, et al., 2012). We found that females tended to release more eggs in the presence of males of the random-harvested line in a group spawning context (two females and four males). Males of the random-harvested line could be preferred by size-selected females and therefore receive more eggs because they were, on average, less aggressive in dyadic contexts compared to males of the large- and small-harvested lines. The presence of harassing males during courtship can increase female reproductive costs and may result in fitness costs to both males and females (Bierbach, Sassmannshausen, Streit, Arias-Rodriguez, & Plath, 2013; Qvarnstrom & Forsgren, 1998; Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2018). In zebrafish, courtship behaviour is usually characterized by a chemical inhibition of male aggression towards females (Hurk & Lambert, 1983; Spence et al., 2008), and female reproductive allocation has been found to be reduced in the presence of extreme large males (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2018; Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, et al., 2012). It is possible that the small boxes and the maintenance of small groups over the four-day period artificially elevated male aggression, in turn reducing the total number of eggs received by males from the large- and small-harvested lines.
We found that males from the random-harvested line were not always less aggressive than males from the other lines; in fact, their aggression increased when paired with females from the large-harvested line without a consistent decrease in the total number of eggs released by females. Thus, it remains unclear whether the increased reproductive allocation experienced by males of the random-harvested line was primarily driven by their lower aggression level or resulted from a combined effect of reduced aggression and other unmeasured male traits (e.g. coloration; Vargas et al., 2018). Moreover, we found that the number of eggs released by females from the large-harvested line was greater than that of females from the other two lines, which could have contributed to our finding that males from the random-harvested line were more aggressive without reducing reproductive output when paired with females from the large-harvested line. An explanation for the elevated number of eggs produced by females from the large-harvested line could be that harvesting triggered the evolution of a fast life history coupled with enhanced reproductive performance early in life (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015; see also Figure 1 and Appendix S1), and females in turn released a greater number of eggs in order to reap fitness benefits early in life to offset the increased probability of adult mortality due to harvest.
We found that, when controlling for length, individuals from the large-harvested line (which represents the typical fisheries scenario with positive size-selective mortality) displayed enhanced reproductive performance (i.e. higher number and proportion of fertilized eggs), independent of the other line that was involved in the spawning. These results can be interpreted as evidence for elevated reproductive fitness of the large-harvested line, which represents the life-history adaptation expected for exploitation contexts wherein large fish are selectively harvested (Andersen, Marty, & Arlinghaus, 2018; Jørgensen et al., 2007). Different fertilization rates in relation to personality and size have been previously documented in zebrafish (e.g. Ariyomo & Watt, 2012, Vargas et al., 2018), which can be both a function of females releasing eggs of differing quality based on perceived male quality and male performance (Ariyomo & Watt, 2012; Uusi-Heikkilä, Böckenhoff, et al., 2012; Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, et al., 2012). Both female and male traits are likely under selection and could have responded to five generations of intensive large size-selective harvesting (Baulier, Morgan, Lilly, Dieckmann, & Heino, 2017; Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015), providing two main explanations for our results. Firstly, as reported previously for these selection lines, the relative fecundity of females of the large-harvested line has been found to be the highest of all selection lines, indicating enhanced reproductive allocation (Uusi-Heikkilä et al., 2015). Similarly, we found that females of the large-harvested line deposited more eggs, which can lead to a higher number of fertilized eggs compared to the other lines. However, female line did not strongly affect the fertilization rate; thus, our results also suggest a further life-history adaptation by males of the large-harvested line in terms of ability to fertilize eggs. For example, males could have evolved specific traits that have been demonstrated to affect fertilization rate in zebrafish, such as keeping proximity to females during spawning contexts (Skinner, 2004), elevated sperm quality (Paull et al., 2008), or improved ability to attract, guard and fertilize eggs via better manoeuvrability during spawning (Vargas et al., 2018; Watt et al., 2011).
Our results align with two studies that tested life-history theory in response to size-selective harvesting in both experimental and field settings. Baulier et al. (2017) found that male (and, to a lesser extent, female) reproductive allocation increased during periods of high fishing mortality in Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua). Moreover, results from a size selection experiment on guppies suggest that changes in reproductive allocation were particularly evident in males, but not in females (Diaz Pauli, Kolding, Jeyakanth, & Heino, 2017). Thus, the enhanced reproductive performance of the large-harvested line is likely caused by the evolution of a fast life history that increased fecundity in females (i.e. number of eggs produced) and fertilization success in males (i.e. number and proportion of eggs fertilized). This interpretation is contingent on the assumption that the F11 lines used herein maintained key life-history adaptations to size-selective harvest observed in previous generations. We show that there is strong evidence to support this claim because the growth trajectories (and thus the underlying life-history traits related to reproduction, energy allocation and timing of maturation) reported by Uusi-Heikkilä et al. (2015) were maintained in the F13 generation (Figure 1). Accordingly, the large-harvested line can be characterized as having a fast life history (i.e. high reproductive allocation and reduced post-maturation growth) and the small-harvested line as having a slow life history (i.e. low reproductive allocation and a large terminal length).
It is possible that some amount of recovery in traits that we did not measure has taken place after F5 when we stopped the size selection, and that such recovery could have influenced some of our findings. Nevertheless, our results suggest that the zebrafish selection lines maintained the previously observed evolved differences in mating behaviour, reproductive performance and life-history six generations after harvesting was halted. This aligns with selection experiments in other fishes that reported only partial recovery of phenotypic traits after a period of highly intensive harvesting (Conover et al., 2009; Salinas et al., 2012). Another core aspect of our work that can be considered both as a strength and as a limitation is that we used size-matched males and females. We did this in an effort to disentangle the effect of a behaviourally mediated mechanism from size effects. However, in zebrafish and in many other species, body size is a key trait under selection (Pyron, 2003; Uusi-Heikkilä, Böckenhoff, et al., 2012; Uusi-Heikkilä, Kuparinen, et al., 2012). It is possible that the adaptations seen in our experiment in the large-harvested line could be reversed when the generally smaller individuals of this line meet with the larger members of the other two lines. Female preferences for larger males could then potentially reinforce fisheries-induced selection towards a fast life history and small adult size as previously argued (Hutchings & Rowe, 2008; Sordalen et al., 2018), rather than fostering population recovery via the enhanced reproductive performance of the large-harvested line documented here when body length was controlled. Further experiments without size-matched fishes have been initiated in our research group to explore this important question and to fully understand how the interaction between behaviourally and size-dependent mate choices could contribute to the evolution of reproductive isolation of exploited populations in different harvesting scenarios.
Given that the selection lines we studied evolved different terminal sizes (Figure 1), the standardization of size across selection lines implies that we used fish from the extremes of the size distributions of the different selection lines (upper percentiles for the large-harvested line and lower percentiles for the small-harvested line). Size variation within a cohort correlates with personality in zebrafish (Polverino, Bierbach, Killen, Uusi-Heikkila, & Arlinghaus, 2016). Therefore, we may have sampled different personalities from the selection lines, thereby possibly confounding our results. However, in a recent study we showed that the selection lines used here differ in personality traits (Sbragaglia, Alós, et al., 2019), but size variation across different cohorts did not explain this personality variation. Therefore, it is unlikely that the size-matched approach used herein was substantially influenced by personality-biased sampling.
5 CONCLUSIONS
Trait and population recovery (e.g. biomass recovery) following intensive size-selective harvesting have been repeatedly shown to be delayed due to FIE (Dunlop, Eikeset, & Stenseth, 2015; Enberg et al., 2009; Neubauer, Jensen, Hutchings, & Baum, 2013). However, most of the published models have not explicitly considered mechanisms of sexual selection, which have the potential to strongly influence FIE and its outcomes (Hutchings & Rowe, 2008; Sordalen et al., 2018). Our results suggest that behaviourally mediated reproductive isolation is unlikely in both positive and negative size-selective harvesting scenarios in the context of the model species that we examined. Furthermore, positive size-selective harvesting can lead to enhanced reproductive performance that, coupled with the absence of behaviourally mediated reproductive isolation, could foster population recovery when exploited populations come into secondary contact with other populations (i.e. through stocking or dispersal from no-take reserves; Alós et al., 2014, Hessenauer et al., 2017). Because our conclusions are based on laboratory experiments on a single model species, we recommend further research including body size variation as well as multiple species comparisons to fully understand the interactions of fisheries-induced changes in mating behaviour and its impacts on sexual selection, reproductive isolation and population recovery.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank all technical helpers during fish breeding and maintenance and reviewers for excellent feedback. All procedures were approved by the Animal Experiment Board in Finland. The authors declare no competing interests.
AUTHORS’ CONTRIBUTIONS
R.A. and D.B. conceived and designed the experiment; C.G. and S.U.-H. acquired the data; V.S., C.G., A.E.H. and D.B. analysed the data; V.S., R.A., D.B. and S.U.-H. interpreted the data; V.S. and R.A. wrote the article with inputs by all other co-authors; all authors gave final approval of the version to be submitted.
Open Research
DATA ACCESSIBILITY
Data are available on Dryad Digital Repository https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.181154k (Sbragaglia et al., 2019).